Music and emotion  

From The Art and Popular Culture Encyclopedia

(Difference between revisions)
Jump to: navigation, search
Revision as of 19:09, 18 December 2019
Jahsonic (Talk | contribs)

← Previous diff
Revision as of 19:12, 18 December 2019
Jahsonic (Talk | contribs)

Next diff →
Line 2: Line 2:
The study of ''''music and emotion'''' seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human [[Affect (psychology)|affect]] and [[music]]. It is a branch of [[music psychology]] with numerous areas of study, including the nature of emotional reactions to music, how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt, and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions. The field draws upon, and has significant implications for, such areas as [[philosophy]], [[musicology]], [[music therapy]], [[music theory]] and [[aesthetics]], as well as the acts of [[musical composition]] and [[performance]]. The study of ''''music and emotion'''' seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human [[Affect (psychology)|affect]] and [[music]]. It is a branch of [[music psychology]] with numerous areas of study, including the nature of emotional reactions to music, how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt, and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions. The field draws upon, and has significant implications for, such areas as [[philosophy]], [[musicology]], [[music therapy]], [[music theory]] and [[aesthetics]], as well as the acts of [[musical composition]] and [[performance]].
-{{TOC limit}} 
- 
-==Philosophical approaches== 
- 
-===Appearance emotionalism === 
-Two of the most influential philosophers in the aesthetics of music are [[Stephen Davies (philosopher)|Stephen Davies]] and [[Jerrold Levinson]].<ref>Cf. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/music/</ref><ref>Davies, S. (2005). "Artistic Expression and the Hard Case of Pure Music", in: Kieran, M. (Ed.), ''Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art'': 179-91.</ref> Davies calls his view of the expressiveness of emotions in music "appearance emotionalism", which holds that music expresses emotion without feeling it. Objects can convey emotion because their structures can contain certain characteristics that resemble emotional expression. "The resemblance that counts most for music's expressiveness ... is between music's temporally unfolding [[dynamic structure]] and configurations of human behaviour associated with the expression of emotion."<ref>Davies 2006, p. 181.</ref> The observer can note emotions from the listener's posture, gait, gestures, attitude, and comportment.<ref>Davies 2006, p. 182.</ref> 
- 
-Associations between musical features and emotion differ among individuals. Appearance emotionalism claims many listeners' perceiving associations constitutes the expressiveness of music. Which musical features are more commonly associated with which emotions is part of [[music psychology]]. Davies claims that expressiveness is an objective property of music and not subjective in the sense of being projected into the music by the listener. Music's expressiveness is certainly response-dependent, i.e. it is realized in the listener's judgement. Skilled listeners very similarly attribute emotional expressiveness to a certain piece of music, thereby indicating according to Davies (2006) that the expressiveness of music is somewhat objective because if the music lacked expressiveness, then no expression could be projected into it as a reaction to the music. 
- 
-===The process theory=== 
-The philosopher [[Jenefer Robinson]]<ref>Jenefer Robinson, Deeper than Reason: Emotion and its Role in Literature, Music, and Art, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005; pp. 310-13</ref> assumes the existence of a mutual dependence between cognition and elicitation in her description of 'emotions as process, music as process' theory (or 'process' theory). Robinson argues that the process of emotional elicitation begins with an 'automatic, immediate response that initiates motor and autonomic activity and prepares us for possible action' causing a process of cognition that may enable listeners to 'name' the felt emotion. This series of events continually exchanges with new, incoming information. Robinson argues that emotions may transform into one another, causing blends, conflicts, and ambiguities that make impede describing with one word the emotional state that one experiences at any given moment; instead, inner feelings are better thought of as the products of multiple emotional 'streams'. Robinson argues that music is a series of simultaneous processes, and that it therefore is an ideal medium for mirroring such more 'cognitive' aspects of emotion as musical themes' 'desiring' resolution or leitmotif's mirrors memory processes. These simultaneous musical processes can reinforce or conflict with each other and thus also express the way one emotion 'morphs into another over time'. 
- 
-==Conveying emotion through music== 
-The ability to perceive emotion in music is said to develop early in childhood, and improve significantly throughout development.<ref name=Foundations>{{cite journal |last= Dowling |first=W.J. |title=The development of music perception and cognition |journal= Foundations of Cognitive Psychology: Core Reading |year=2002 |pages= 481–502}}</ref> The capacity to perceive emotion in music is also subject to cultural influences, and both similarities and differences in emotion perception have been observed in cross-cultural studies.<ref name="Susino and Schubert, 2017">{{cite journal |last=Susino |first=M. |author2=Schubert, S. |title= Cross-cultural anger communication in music: Towards a stereotype theory of emotion in music |journal=Musicae Scientiae |volume=21 |year=2017 |pages= 60–74|doi=10.1177/1029864916637641 }}</ref><ref name="Thompson 2010">{{cite book |last1=Thompson |first1=William Forde |last2=Balkwill |first2=Laura-Lee |editor1-last=Juslin |editor1-first=Patrik |editor2-last=Sloboda |editor2-first=John |title= Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=2010 |pages= 755–788 |chapter= Chapter 27: Cross-cultural similarities and differences |chapterurl= http://www.psy.mq.edu.au/me2/uploads/publications/27-Juslin__Sloboda-Ch-27.pdf |isbn=978-0-19-960496-8 |lastauthoramp=y}}</ref> Empirical research has looked at which emotions can be conveyed as well as what structural factors in music help contribute to the perceived emotional expression. There are two schools of thought on how we interpret emotion in music. The [[Cognitivism (psychology)|cognitivists']] approach argues that music simply displays an emotion, but does not allow for the personal experience of emotion in the listener. [[Emotivism|Emotivists]] argue that music elicits real emotional responses in the listener.<ref name=Chapter16>{{cite journal|last=Scherer|first=K. R.|author2=Zentner, M. R.|title=Emotional effects of music: production rules|journal=Music and Emotion: Theory and Research|year=2001|pages=361–387}}</ref><ref name=Radford>{{cite journal|last=Radford|first=C.|title=Emotions and music: A reply to the cognitivists|journal=The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism|year=1989|volume=47|issue=1|pages=69–76|jstor=431994|doi=10.2307/431994}}</ref> 
- 
-It has been argued that the emotion experienced from a piece of music is a multiplicative function of structural features, performance features, listener features and contextual features of the piece, shown as: 
- 
-:''Experienced Emotion'' = ''Structural features'' × ''Performance features'' × ''Listener features'' × ''Contextual features'' 
- 
-where: 
- 
-:''Structural features'' = Segmental features × Suprasegmental features 
-:''Performance features'' = Performer skill × Performer state 
-:''Listener features'' = Musical expertise × Stable disposition × Current motivation 
-:''Contextual features'' = Location × Event<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-===Structural features=== 
-Structural features are divided into two parts, segmental features and suprasegmental features. Segmental features are the individual sounds or tones that make up the music; this includes acoustic structures such as [[Duration (music)|duration]], [[amplitude]], and [[pitch (music)|pitch]]. Suprasegmental features are the foundational structures of a piece, such as [[melody]], [[tempo]] and [[rhythm]].<ref name="Chapter16" /> There are a number of specific musical features that are highly associated with particular emotions.<ref name="Chapter10">{{cite journal |last=Gabrielle |first=A. |last2=Stromboli |first2=E. |title=The influence of musical structure on emotional expression |journal=Music and Emotion: Theory and Research |year=2001 |pages=223–243}}</ref> Within the factors affecting emotional expression in music, [[tempo]] is typically regarded as the most important, but a number of other factors, such as [[mode (music)|mode]], [[loudness]], and [[melody]], also influence the emotional valence of the piece.<ref name="Chapter10"/> 
- 
-{| class="wikitable" 
-|- 
-! Structural Feature !! Definition !! Associated Emotions 
-|- 
-| Tempo || The speed or pace of a musical piece || Fast tempo:happiness, excitement, anger. Slow tempo: sadness, serenity. 
-|- 
-| Mode || The type of scale || Major tonality: happiness, joy. Minor tonality: sadness. 
-|- 
-| Loudness || The physical strength and amplitude of a sound || Intensity, power, or anger 
-|- 
-| Melody || The linear succession of musical tones that the listener perceives as a single entity || Complementing harmonies: happiness, relaxation, serenity. Clashing harmonies: excitement, anger, unpleasantness. 
-|- 
-| Rhythm || The regularly recurring pattern or beat of a song || Smooth/consistent rhythm: happiness, peace. Rough/irregular rhythm: amusement, uneasiness. Varied rhythm: joy. 
-|} 
- 
-Some studies find that perception of basic emotional features are a [[cultural universal]], though people can more easily perceive emotion, and perceive more nuanced emotion, in music from their own culture.<ref>[https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/talking-apes/201507/is-music-universal-language Is Music a Universal Language?]</ref><ref>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4286616/ Music induces universal emotion-related psychophysiological responses: comparing Canadian listeners to Congolese Pygmies]</ref><ref>[https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0305735615589214 Perception of basic emotions in music: Culture-specific or multicultural?]</ref> 
- 
-Music has a direct connection to emotional states present in human beings. Different musical structures have been found to have a relationship with physiological responses. Research has shown that suprasegmental structures such as tonal space, specifically dissonance, create unpleasant negative emotions in participants. The emotional responses were measured with physiological assessments, such as skin conductance and electromyographic signals (EMG), while participants listened to musical excerpts.<ref>Dellacherie, D., Roy, M., Hugueville, L., Peretz, I., & Samson, S. (2011). The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance. Psychophysiology, 48(3), 337-349. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01075.x</ref> Further research on psychophysiological measures pertaining to music were conducted and found similar results; musical structures of rhythmic articulation, accentuation, and tempo were found to correlate strongly with physiological measures, the measured used here included heart rate and respiratory monitors that correlated with self-report questionnaires.<ref>Gomez, P., & Danuser, B. (2007). Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion, 7(2), 377-387. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377</ref> 
-  
-Music also affects socially-relevant memories, specifically memories produced by nostalgic musical excerpts (e.g., music from a significant time period in one’s life, like music listened to on road trips). Musical structures are more strongly interpreted in certain areas of the brain when the music evokes nostalgia. The interior frontal gyrus, substantia nigra, cerebellum, and insula were all identified to have a stronger correlation with nostalgic music than not.<ref>Barrett, F. S., & Janata, P. (2016). Neural responses to nostalgia-evoking music modeled by elements of dynamic musical structure and individual differences in affective traits. Neuropsychologia, 91, 234-246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.012 
-</ref> Brain activity is a very individualized concept with many of the musical excerpts having certain effects based on individuals’ past life experiences, thus this caveat should be kept in mind when generalizing findings across individuals. 
- 
-===Performance features=== 
-Performance features refer to the manner in which a piece of music is executed by the performer(s). These are broken into two categories, performer skills, and performer state. Performer skills are the compound ability and appearance of the performer; including physical appearance, reputation, and technical skills. The performer state is the interpretation, motivation, and stage presence of the performer.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-=== Listener features === 
-Listener features refer to the individual and social identity of the listener(s). This includes their personality, age, knowledge of music, and motivation to listen to the music.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-===Contextual features=== 
-Contextual features are aspects of the performance such as the location and the particular occasion for the performance (i.e., funeral, wedding, dance).<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-These different factors influence expressed emotion at different magnitudes, and their effects are compounded by one another. Thus, experienced emotion is felt to a stronger degree if more factors are present. The order the factors are listed within the model denotes how much weight in the equation they carry. For this reason, the bulk of research has been done in structural features and listener features.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-====Conflicting cues==== 
-Which emotion is perceived is dependent on the context of the piece of music. Past research has argued that opposing emotions like happiness and sadness fall on a bipolar scale, where both cannot be felt at the same time.<ref name=Hunter2010>{{cite journal|last=Hunter|first=P. G.|author2=Schellenburg, E. G. |author3=Schimmack, U. |title=Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: Similarities, differences, and mixed emotions|journal=Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts|year=2010|volume=4|pages=47–56|doi=10.1037/a0016873|url=https://semanticscholar.org/paper/2d9136dab4265cbd366ec474e0f7ec0641541246}}</ref> More recent research has suggested that happiness and sadness are experienced separately, which implies that they can be felt concurrently.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> One study investigated the latter possibility by having participants listen to computer-manipulated musical excerpts that have mixed cues between tempo and mode.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> Examples of mix-cue music include a piece with major key and slow tempo, and a minor-chord piece with a fast tempo. Participants then rated the extent to which the piece conveyed happiness or sadness. The results indicated that mixed-cue music conveys both happiness and sadness; however, it remained unclear whether participants perceived happiness and sadness simultaneously or vacillated between these two emotions.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> A follow-up study was done to examine these possibilities. While listening to mixed or consistent cue music, participants pressed one button when the music conveyed happiness, and another button when it conveyed sadness.<ref name=Larsen /> The results revealed that subjects pressed both buttons simultaneously during songs with conflicting cues.<ref name=Larsen>{{cite journal|last=Larsen|first=J. T.|author2=Stastny, B. J.|title=It's a bittersweet symphony: Simultaneously mixed emotional responses to music with conflicting cues|journal=Emotion|year=2011|doi=10.1037/a0024081|pmid=21707144|volume=11|issue=6|pages=1469–1473}}</ref> These findings indicate that listeners can perceive both happiness and sadness concurrently. This has significant implications for how the structural features influence emotion, because when a mix of structural cues is used, a number of emotions may be conveyed.<ref name=Larsen /> 
- 
-===Specific listener features=== 
- 
-====Development==== 
-Studies indicate that the ability to understand emotional messages in music starts early, and improves throughout child development.<ref name=Foundations /><ref name=Chapter10 /><ref name=Chapter4>{{cite journal|last=Sloboda|first=J. A.|author2=Juslin, P. N.|title=Psychological perspectives on music and emotion|journal=Music and Emotion: Theory and Research|year=2001|pages=79–96}}</ref> Studies investigating music and emotion in children primarily play a musical excerpt for children and have them look at pictorial expressions of faces. These facial expressions display different emotions and children are asked to select the face that best matches the music's emotional tone.<ref name=Nawrot>{{cite journal|last=Nawrot|first=E. S.|title=The Perception of Emotional Expression in Music: Evidence from Infants, Children and Adults|journal=Psychology of Music|date=1 January 2003|volume=31|issue=1|pages=75–92|doi=10.1177/0305735603031001325}}</ref><ref name=Mote>{{cite journal|last=Mote|first=Jasmine|title=The effects of tempo and familiarity on children's affective interpretation of music.|journal=Emotion|date=1 January 2011|volume=11|issue=3|pages=618–622|doi=10.1037/a0022573|pmid=21668112}}</ref><ref name=Terwogt>{{cite journal|last=TERWOGT|first=MARK MEERUM|author2=VAN GRINSVEN, FLORA|journal=Perceptual and Motor Skills|date=1 December 1988|volume=67|issue=3|pages=697–698|doi=10.2466/pms.1988.67.3.697|pmid=3226819|title=Recognition of Emotions in Music by Children and Adults}}</ref> Studies have shown that children are able to assign specific emotions to pieces of music; however, there is debate regarding the age at which this ability begins.<ref name=Foundations /><ref name=Chapter10 /><ref name=Chapter4 /> 
- 
-=====''Infants''===== 
-An infant is often exposed to a mother's speech that is musical in nature. It is possible that the motherly singing allows the mother to relay emotional messages to the infant.<ref name=Trehub>{{cite journal |last=Trehub |first=Sandra E. |author2=Nakata, Takayuki |title=Emotion and music in infancy |journal= Musicae Scientiae |year=2001 |volume=SPEC ISSUE, 2001-2002 |pages= 37–61|doi=10.1177/10298649020050S103 }}</ref> Infants also tend to prefer positive speech to neutral speech as well as happy music to negative music.<ref name=Nawrot /><ref name=Trehub /> It has also been posited that listening to their mother's singing may play a role in identity formation.<ref name=Trehub /> This hypothesis is supported by a study that interviewed adults and asked them to describe musical experiences from their childhood. Findings showed that music was good for developing knowledge of emotions during childhood.<ref name=Vist>{{cite journal|last=Vist|first=Torill|title=Music Experience in Early Childhood: Potential for Emotion Knowledge?|journal=International Journal of Early Childhood|date=16 November 2011|volume=43|issue=3|pages=277–290|doi=10.1007/s13158-011-0045-7}}</ref> 
- 
-=====''Pre-school children''===== 
-These studies have shown that children at the age of 4 are able to begin to distinguish between emotions found in musical excerpts in ways that are similar to adults.<ref name=Nawrot /><ref name=Mote /> The ability to distinguish these musical emotions seems to increase with age until adulthood.<ref name=Terwogt /> However, children at the age of 3 were unable to make the distinction between emotions expressed in music through matching a facial expression with the type of emotion found in the music.<ref name=Mote /> Some emotions, such as anger and fear, were also found to be harder to distinguish within music.<ref name=Terwogt /><ref name=Robazza>{{cite journal|last=ROBAZZA|first=CLAUDIO |author2=MACALUSO, CRISTINA |author3=D'URSO, VALENTINA|journal=Perceptual and Motor Skills|date=1 October 1994|volume=79|issue=2|pages=939–944|doi=10.2466/pms.1994.79.2.939|pmid=7870518 |title=Emotional Reactions to Music by Gender, Age, and Expertise }}</ref> 
- 
-=====''Elementary-age children''===== 
-In studies with four-year-olds and five-year-olds, they are asked to label musical excerpts with the affective labels "happy", "sad", "angry", and "afraid".<ref name=Foundations /> Results in one study showed that four-year-olds did not perform above chance with the labels "sad" and "angry", and the five-year-olds did not perform above chance with the label "afraid".<ref name=Foundations /> A follow-up study found conflicting results, where five-year-olds performed much like adults. However, all ages confused categorizing "angry" and "afraid".<ref name=Foundations /> 
-Pre-school and elementary-age children listened to twelve short melodies, each in either major or minor mode, and were instructed to choose between four pictures of faces: happy, contented, sad, and angry.<ref name=Chapter10 /> All the children, even as young as three years old, performed above chance in assigning positive faces with major mode and negative faces with minor mode.<ref name=Chapter10 /> 
- 
-====Personality effects==== 
-Different people perceive events differently based upon their individual characteristics. Similarly, the emotions elicited by listening to different types of music seem to be affected by factors such as personality and previous musical training.<ref name=Ladinig>{{cite journal|last=Ladinig|first=Olivia|author2=Schellenberg, E. Glenn|title=Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences.|journal=Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts|date=1 January 2012|volume=6|issue=2|pages=146–154|doi=10.1037/a0024671}}</ref><ref name=Schellenberg>{{cite journal|last=Schellenberg|first=E. Glenn|author2=Mankarious, Monika|title=Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood.|journal=Emotion|date=1 January 2012|volume=12|issue=5|pages=887–891|doi=10.1037/a0027971|pmid=22642351}}</ref><ref name="Vuoskoski Measure">{{cite journal|last=Vuoskoski|first=J. K.|author2=Eerola, T.|title=Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences|journal=Musicae Scientiae|date=13 July 2011|volume=15|issue=2|pages=159–173|doi=10.1177/1029864911403367}}</ref> People with the personality type of agreeableness have been found to have higher emotional responses to music in general. Stronger sad feelings have also been associated with people with personality types of agreeableness and neuroticism. While some studies have shown that musical training can be correlated with music that evoked mixed feelings<ref name=Ladinig /> as well as higher IQ and test of emotional comprehension scores,<ref name=Schellenberg /> other studies refute the claim that musical training affects perception of emotion in music.<ref name=Robazza /><ref name=Kratus>{{cite journal|last=Kratus|first=J.|title=A Developmental Study of Children's Interpretation of Emotion in Music|journal=Psychology of Music|date=1 January 1993|volume=21|issue=1|pages=3–19|doi=10.1177/030573569302100101}}</ref> It is also worth noting that previous exposure to music can affect later behavioral choices, schoolwork, and social interactions.<ref name="Policy Statement">{{cite journal|title=Impact of Music, Music Lyrics, and Music Videos on Children and Youth|journal=Pediatrics|date=19 October 2009|volume=124|issue=5|pages=1488–1494|doi=10.1542/peds.2009-2145|pmid=19841124|author1=Council on Communications Media}}</ref> Therefore, previous music exposure does seem to have an effect on the personality and emotions of a child later in their life, and would subsequently affect their ability to perceive as well as express emotions during exposure to music. Gender, however, has not been shown to lead to a difference in perception of emotions found in music.<ref name=Robazza /><ref name=Kratus /> Further research into which factors affect an individual's perception of emotion in music and the ability of the individual to have music-induced emotions are needed. 
- 
-==Eliciting emotion through music== 
-Along with the research that music conveys an emotion to its listener(s), it has also been shown that music can produce emotion in the listener(s).<ref name=Garrido>{{cite journal|last=Garrido|first=S.|author2=E. Shubert|title=Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: a literature review and experiment|journal=Music Perception|year=2011|volume=28|issue=3|pages=279–295|doi=10.1525/MP.2011.28.3.279}}</ref> This view often causes debate because the emotion is produced within the listener, and is consequently hard to measure. In spite of controversy, studies have shown observable responses to elicited emotions, which reinforces the Emotivists' view that music does elicit real emotional responses.<ref name=Foundations /><ref name=Radford /> 
- 
-===Responses to elicited emotion=== 
-The structural features of music not only help convey an emotional message to the listener, but also may create emotion in the listener.<ref name=Chapter16 /> These emotions can be completely new feelings or may be an extension of previous emotional events. Empirical research has shown how listeners can absorb the piece's expression as their own emotion, as well as invoke a unique response based on their personal experiences.<ref name=Chapter4 /> 
- 
-====''Basic emotions''==== 
-In research on eliciting emotion, participants report personally feeling a certain emotion in response to hearing a musical piece.<ref name=Garrido /> Researchers have investigated whether the same structures that conveyed a particular emotion could elicit it as well. The researchers presented excerpts of fast tempo, major mode music and slow tempo, minor tone music to participants; these musical structures were chosen because they are known to convey happiness and sadness respectively.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> Participants rated their own emotions with elevated levels of happiness after listening to music with structures that convey happiness and elevated sadness after music with structures that convey sadness.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> This evidence suggests that the same structures that convey emotions in music can also elicit those same emotions in the listener. 
- 
-In light of this finding, there has been particular controversy about music eliciting negative emotions. [[Cognitivism (psychology)|Cognitivists]] argue that choosing to listen to music that elicits negative emotions like sadness would be paradoxical, as listeners would not willingly strive to induce sadness.<ref name=Radford /> However, [[Emotivism|emotivists]] purport that music does elicit negative emotions, and listeners knowingly choose to listen in order to feel sadness in an impersonal way, similar to a viewer's desire to watch a tragic film.<ref name=Radford /><ref name=Garrido /> The reasons why people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad has been explored by means of interviewing people about their motivations for doing so. As a result of this research it has indeed been found that people sometimes listen to sad music when feeling sad to intensify feelings of sadness. Other reasons for listening to sad music when feeling sad were; in order to retrieve memories, to feel closer to other people, for [[cognitive reappraisal]], to feel befriended by the music, to distract oneself, and for mood enhancement.<ref name="Van den Tol">{{cite journal|last=Van den TOl|first=A.J.M.|author2=Edwards, J.|title=Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad. |journal=Psychology of Music|year=2013}}</ref>  
-  
-Researchers have also found an effect between one's familiarity with a piece of music and the emotions it elicits.<ref name=Ali>{{cite journal|last=Ali|first=S. O.|author2=Peynircioglu, Z. F.|title=Intensity of emotions conveyed and elicited by familiar and unfamiliar music|journal=Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal|year=2010|volume=27|issue=3|pages=177–182|jstor=10.1525/mp.2010.27.3.177|doi=10.1525/MP.2010.27.3.177}}</ref> In one study, half of participants were played twelve random musical excerpts one time, and rated their emotions after each piece. The other half of the participants listened to twelve random excerpts five times, and started their ratings on the third repetition. Findings showed that participants who listened to the excerpts five times rated their emotions with higher intensity than the participants who listened to them only once.<ref name=Ali /> This suggests that familiarity with a piece of music increases the emotions experienced by the listener. 
- 
-====''Emotional memories and actions''==== 
-Music may not only elicit new emotions, but connect listeners with other emotional sources.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Music serves as a powerful cue to recall emotional memories back into awareness.<ref>Van den Tol, A. J. M., & Ritchie, T. D.(book-chapter in press). Emotion memory and music: A critical review and recommendations for future research.Music, Memory and Autobiography. (Eds: Professor Strollo Maria Rosaria and Dr. Romano Alessandra).</ref> Because music is such a pervasive part of social life, present in weddings, funerals and religious ceremonies, it brings back emotional memories that are often already associated with it.<ref name=Chapter16 /><ref name=Chapter4 /> Music is also processed by the lower, sensory levels of the brain, making it impervious to later memory distortions. Therefore creating a strong connection between emotion and music within memory makes it easier to recall one when prompted by the other.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Music can also tap into empathy, inducing emotions that are assumed to be felt by the performer or composer. Listeners can become sad because they recognize that those emotions must have been felt by the composer,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Van | last2 = den Tol | first2 = A. J. M. | last3 = Edwards | first3 = J. | year = 2013 | title = Exploring a rationale for choosing to listen to sad music when feeling sad | url = http://eprints.lincoln.ac.uk/33981/3/prepublished%20-%20rationale%20for%20sad%20music.doc| journal = Psychology of Music | volume = 41 | issue = 4| pages = 440–465 | doi = 10.1177/0305735611430433}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Van | last2 = den Tol | first2 = A. J. M. | last3 = Edwards | first3 = J. | year = 2014 | title = Listening to sad music in adverse situations: Music selection strategies, self-regulatory goals, listening effect, and mood-enhancement | url = http://eprints.lincoln.ac.uk/33982/1/listeningtosad-post-2014.pdf| journal = Psychology of Music | volume = 43| issue = 4| pages = 473–494| doi = 10.1177/0305735613517410 }}</ref> much as the viewer of a play can empathize for the actors. 
- 
-Listeners may also respond to emotional music through action.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Throughout history music was composed to inspire people into specific action - to march, dance, sing or fight. Consequently, heightening the emotions in all these events. In fact, many people report being unable to sit still when certain rhythms are played, in some cases even engaging in subliminal actions when physical manifestations should be suppressed.<ref name=Chapter4 /> Examples of this can be seen in young children's spontaneous outbursts into motion upon hearing music, or exuberant expressions shown at concerts.<ref name=Chapter4 /> 
- 
-====Juslin & Västfjäll's BRECVEM model==== 
- 
-Juslin & Västfjäll<ref>Patrik Juslin & Daniel V ̈astfj ̈all, 'Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 2008; 559-621.</ref><ref>Juslin, Liljeström, Västfjäll, & Lundqvist. (2010). How does music evoke emotions? Exploring the underlying mechanisms. In P.N. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), ''Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications'' (pp. 605-642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> developed a model of seven ways in which music can elicit emotion, called the BRECVEM model. 
- 
-''Brain Stem Reflex'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by music because one or more fundamental acoustical characteristics of the music are taken by the brain stem to signal a potentially important and urgent event. All other things being equal, sounds that are sudden, loud, dissonant, or feature fast temporal patterns induce arousal or feelings of unpleasantness in listeners...Such responses reflect the impact of auditory sensations – music as sound in the most basic sense.' 
- 
-''Rhythmic Entrainment'':<ref>Juslin, P. (2011). Music and Emotion: seven questions, seven answers. In I. Deliège, J. Davidson, J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), ''Music and the Mind: Essays in Honour of John Sloboda'' (pp.113-138). Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is evoked by a piece of music because a powerful, external rhythm in the music influences some internal bodily rhythm of the listener (e.g. heart rate), such that the latter rhythm adjusts toward and eventually 'locks in' to a common periodicity. The adjusted heart rate can then spread to other components of emotion such as feeling, through proprioceptive feedback. This may produce an increased level of arousal in the listener.' 
- 
-''Evaluative Conditioning'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music simply because this stimulus has been paired repeatedly with other positive or negative stimuli. Thus, for instance, a particular piece of music may have occurred repeatedly together in time with a specific event that always made you happy (e.g., meeting your best friend). Over time, through repeated pairings, the music will eventually come to evoke happiness even in the absence of the friendly interaction.' 
- 
-''Emotional Contagion'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced by a piece of music because the listener perceives the emotional expression of the music, and then "mimics" this expression internally, which by means of either peripheral feedback from muscles, or a more direct activation of the relevant emotional representations in the brain, leads to an induction of the same emotion.' 
- 
-''Visual Imagery'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because he or she conjures up visual images (e.g., of a beautiful landscape) while listening to the music.' 
- 
-''Episodic memory'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because the music evokes a memory of a particular event in the listener's life. This is sometimes referred to as the "Darling, they are playing our tune" phenomenon.'<ref>Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. Hutchinson.</ref> 
- 
-''Musical expectancy'': 'This refers to a process whereby an emotion is induced in a listener because a specific feature of the music violates, delays, or confirms the listener's expectations about the continuation of the music.' 
- 
-====Musical expectancy==== 
- 
-With regards to violations of expectation in music several interesting results have been found. It has for example been found that listening to unconventional music may sometimes cause a meaning threat and result in compensatory behaviour in order to restore meaning.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Maher | first1 = P | last2 = Van Tilburg | first2 = W. A. P. | last3 = Van | last4 = den Tol | first4 = A. J. M. | year = 2013 | title = Meaning in music: Deviations from expectation in music polarize in-group and out-group attitudes | url = | journal = European Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 34| issue = 2| pages = 155–172| doi = 10.1002/ejsp.196 }}</ref> 
- 
-====Aesthetic Judgement and BRECVEMA==== 
- 
-In 2013, Juslin created an additional aspect to the BRECVEM model called aesthetic judgement.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Juslin | first1 = P. N. | year = 2013 | title = From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: towards a unified theory of musical emotions | url = | journal = Physics of Life Reviews | volume = 10 | issue = 3| pages = 235–266 | doi=10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008| pmid = 23769678 | bibcode = 2013PhLRv..10..235J }}</ref> This is the criteria which each individual has as a metric for music's aesthetic value. This can involve a number of varying personal preferences, such as the message conveyed, skill presented or novelty of style or idea. 
- 
-==Comparison of conveyed and elicited emotions== 
- 
-===Evidence for emotion in music=== 
-There has been a bulk of evidence that listeners can identify specific emotions with certain types of music, but there has been less concrete evidence that music may elicit emotions.<ref name=Chapter16 /> This is due to the fact that elicited emotion is subjective; and thus, it is difficult to find a valid criterion to study it.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Elicited and conveyed emotion in music is usually understood from three types of evidence: self-report, physiological responses, and expressive behavior. Researchers use one or a combination of these methods to investigate emotional reactions to music.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-====''Self-report''==== 
-The self-report method is a verbal report by the listener regarding what they are experiencing. This is the most widely used method for studying emotion and has shown that people identify emotions and personally experience emotions while listening to music.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Research in the area has shown that listeners' emotional responses are highly consistent. In fact, a meta-analysis of 41 studies on music performance found that happiness, sadness, tenderness, threat, and anger were identified above chance by listeners.<ref name=Vieillard>{{cite journal|last=Vieillard|first=S.|author2=Peretz, I. |author3=Gosselin, N. |author4=Khalfa, S. |title=Happy, sad, scary, and peaceful musical excerpts for research on emotions|journal=Cognition and Emotion|year=2008|volume=22|issue=4|pages=720–752|doi=10.1080/02699930701503567}}</ref> Another study compared untrained listeners to musically trained listeners.<ref name=Vieillard /> Both groups were required to categorize musical excerpts that conveyed similar emotions. The findings showed that the categorizations were not different between the trained and untrained; thus demonstrating that the untrained listeners are highly accurate in perceiving emotion.<ref name=Vieillard /> It is more difficult to find evidence for elicited emotion, as it depends solely on the subjective response of the listener. This leaves reporting vulnerable to self-report biases such as participants responding according to social prescriptions or responding as they think the experimenter wants them to.<ref name=Chapter16 /> As a result, the validity of the self-report method is often questioned, and consequently researchers are reluctant to draw definitive conclusions solely from these reports.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-====''Physiological responses''==== 
-Emotions are known to create physiological, or bodily, changes in a person, which can be tested experimentally. Some evidence shows one of these changes is within the nervous system.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Arousing music is related to increased heart rate and muscle tension; calming music is connected to decreased heart rate and muscle tension, and increased skin temperature.<ref name=Chapter16 /> Other research identifies outward physical responses such as shivers or goose bumps to be caused by changes in harmony and tears or lump-in-the-throat provoked by changes in melody.<ref name=Chapter19>{{cite journal|last=Gabrielsson|first=A.|title=Emotion in strong experiences with music|journal=Music and Emotion: Theory and Research|year=2001|pages=431–449}}</ref> Researchers test these responses through the use of instruments for physiological measurement, such as recording pulse rate.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-====''Expressive behavior''==== 
-People are also known to show outward manifestations of their emotional states while listening to music. Studies using facial [[electromyography]] (EMG) have found that people react with subliminal facial expressions when listening to expressive music.<ref name=Chapter4 /> In addition, music provides a stimulus for expressive behavior in many social contexts, such as concerts, dances, and ceremonies.<ref name=Chapter16 /><ref name=Chapter4 /> Although these expressive behaviors can be measured experimentally, there have been very few controlled studies observing this behavior.<ref name=Chapter16 /> 
- 
-===Strength of effects=== 
-Within the comparison between elicited and conveyed emotions, researchers have examined the relationship between these two types of responses to music. In general, research agrees that feeling and perception ratings are highly correlated, but not identical.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> More specifically, studies are inconclusive as to whether one response has a stronger effect than the other, and in what ways these two responses relate.<ref name=Hunter2010 /><ref name=Ali /><ref name=Kallenin /> 
- 
-====''Conveyed more than elicited''==== 
-In one study, participants heard a random selection of 24 excerpts, displaying six types of emotions, five times in a row.<ref name=Ali /> Half the participants described the emotions the music conveyed, and the other half responded with how the music made them feel. The results found that emotions conveyed by music were more intense than the emotions elicited by the same piece of music.<ref name=Ali /> Another study investigated under what specific conditions strong emotions were conveyed. Findings showed that ratings for conveyed emotions were higher in happy responses to music with consistent cues for happiness (i.e., fast tempo and major mode), for sad responses to music with consistent cues for sadness (i.e., slow tempo and minor mode,) and for sad responses in general.<ref name=Hunter2010 /> These studies suggest that people can recognize the emotion displayed in music more readily than feeling it personally. 
- 
-====''Sometimes conveyed, sometimes elicited''==== 
-Another study that had 32 participants listen to twelve musical pieces and found that the strength of perceived and elicited emotions were dependent on the structures of the piece of music.<ref name=Kallenin /> Perceived emotions were stronger than felt emotions when listeners rated for arousal and positive and negative activation. On the other hand, elicited emotions were stronger than perceived emotions when rating for pleasantness.<ref name=Kallenin>{{cite journal|last=Kallenin|first=K|author2=Ravaja, N.|title=Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same and different|journal=Musicae Scientiae|year=2006|volume=10|issue=2|pages=191–213|doi=10.1177/102986490601000203}}</ref> 
- 
-====''Elicited more than conveyed''==== 
-In another study analysis revealed that emotional responses were stronger than the listeners' perceptions of emotions.<ref name=Kallenin /> This study used a between-subjects design, where 20 listeners judged to what extent they perceived four emotions: happy, sad, peaceful, and scared. A separate 19 listeners rated to what extent they experienced each of these emotions. The findings showed that all music stimuli elicited specific emotions for the group of participants rating elicited emotion, while music stimuli only occasionally conveyed emotion to the participants in the group identifying which emotions the music conveyed.<ref name=Kallenin />  
-Based on these inconsistent findings, there is much research left to be done in order to determine how conveyed and elicited emotions are similar and different. There is disagreement about whether music induces 'true' emotions or if the emotions reported as felt in studies are instead just participants stating the emotions found in the music they are listening to.<ref name=Schubert>{{cite journal|last=Schubert|first=Emery|title=Locus of emotion: The effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music|journal=Journal of Music Therapy|year=2007|volume=44|issue=4|pages=344–368|doi=10.1093/jmt/44.4.344}}</ref><ref name=Zentner>{{cite journal|last=Zentner|first=Marcel|author2=Grandjean, Didier |author3=Scherer, Klaus R. |title=Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement.|journal=Emotion|date=1 January 2008|volume=8|issue=4|pages=494–521|doi=10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494|citeseerx=10.1.1.459.9866}}</ref> 
- 
-==Music as a therapeutic tool== 
-{{Main|Music therapy}} 
-Music therapy as a therapeutic tool has been shown to be an effective treatment for various ailments. Therapeutic techniques involve eliciting emotions by listening to music, composing music or lyrics and performing music.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Keen MSocSc | first1 = Alexander W. | year = 2005 | title = Using Music as a Therapy Tool to Motivate Troubled Adolescents | url = | journal = Social Work in Health Care | volume = 39 | issue = 3–4| pages = 361–373 | doi = 10.1300/J010v39n03_09 }}</ref> 
-  
-Music therapy sessions may have the ability to help drug users who are attempting to break a drug habit, with users reporting feeling better able to feel emotions without the aid of drug use.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Baker | first1 = Felicity A. | last2 = Gleadhill | first2 = Libby M. | last3 = Dingle | first3 = Genevieve A. | year = 2007 | title = Music therapy and emotional exploration: Exposing substance abuse clients to the experiences of non-drug-induced emotions | journal = The Arts in Psychotherapy | volume = 34 | issue = 4| pages = 321–330 | doi=10.1016/j.aip.2007.04.005}}</ref> Music therapy may also be a viable option for people experiencing extended stays in a hospital due to illness. In one study, music therapy provided child oncology patients with enhanced environmental support elements and elicited more engaging behaviors from the child.<ref name=Robb>{{cite journal|last=Robb|first=Sheri L.|title=The effect of therapeutic music interventions on the behavior of hospitalized children in isolation: Developing a contextual support model of music therapy|journal=Journal of Music Therapy|year=2000|volume=37|issue=2|pages=118–146|doi=10.1093/jmt/37.2.118|pmid=10932125}}</ref> When treating troubled teenagers, a study by Keen revealed that music therapy has allowed therapists to interact with teenagers with less resistance, thus facilitating self-expression in the teenager.{{citation needed|date=December 2014}} 
-  
-Music therapy has also shown great promise in individuals with autism, serving as an emotional outlet for these patients. While other avenues of emotional expression and understanding may be difficult for people with autism, music may provide those with limited understanding of socio-emotional cues a way of accessing emotion.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Heaton | first1 = Pam | year = 2009 | title = Music - shelter for the frazzled mind?". | url = | journal = The Psychologist | volume = 22 | issue = 12| pages = 1018–1020 }}</ref> 
- 
{{GFDL}} {{GFDL}}

Revision as of 19:12, 18 December 2019

Related e

Wikipedia
Wiktionary
Shop


Featured:

The study of 'music and emotion' seeks to understand the psychological relationship between human affect and music. It is a branch of music psychology with numerous areas of study, including the nature of emotional reactions to music, how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt, and which components of a musical composition or performance may elicit certain reactions. The field draws upon, and has significant implications for, such areas as philosophy, musicology, music therapy, music theory and aesthetics, as well as the acts of musical composition and performance.



Unless indicated otherwise, the text in this article is either based on Wikipedia article "Music and emotion" or another language Wikipedia page thereof used under the terms of the GNU Free Documentation License; or on research by Jahsonic and friends. See Art and Popular Culture's copyright notice.

Personal tools